Winter symposium Aarhus (DK)

Slides:



Advertisements
Ähnliche Präsentationen
Word Order in German Subordiante Clauses
Advertisements

H - A - M - L - E - IC T Teachers Acting Patterns while Teaching with New Media in the Subjects German, Mathematics and Computer Science Prof. S. Blömeke,
The difference between kein and nicht.
Personal Pronouns in the Dative Kapitel 10 Lektion B
Wozu die Autokorrelationsfunktion?
Three minutes presentation I ArbeitsschritteW Seminar I-Prax: Inhaltserschließung visueller Medien, Spree WS 2010/2011 Giving directions.
Hochschulteam der Agentur für Arbeit Trier Preventing the Brainware Crisis Workshop Schloss Dagstuhl Student Enrollment in Computer Science.
Introduction to the topic. Goals: Improving the students essay style in general Finding special words and expressions that can be used in essay writing.
Comparatives Meine Stadt ist schön Deine Stadt ist schöner Just add er to the end of the adjective to make it more… e.g. more beautiful.
Kapitel 1 Komm mit! Level I erste Stufe.
Die Hausaufgaben: Machen Sie Ü. 7 auf S. 29
Adjektive Endungen von Frau Templeton.
Zu + Infinitiv : eine Erklärung
Laurie Clarcq The purpose of language, used in communication, is to create a picture in the mind and/or the heart of another.
Institut AIFB, Universität Karlsruhe (TH) Forschungsuniversität gegründet 1825 Towards Automatic Composition of Processes based on Semantic.
| DC-IAP/SVC3 | © Bosch Rexroth Pneumatics GmbH This document, as well as the data, specifications and other information set forth in.
Das Perfekt (Present Perfect Tense). Think of 5 things you did in your holidays but think of sentences in the PRESENT TENSE. 1.Am Montag schlafe ich viel.
Plural Forms of Nouns & Wie viel? or Wie viele?
Starter: Match-up 1.Ich spiele… 2.Ich höre… 3.Ich telefoniere… 4.Ich sehe… 5.Ich surfe … 6.Ich gehe auf … 7.Ich treibe … 8.Ich spiele ein … 9.Ich sammele.
Adjective Endings Frau Lizz Caplan-Carbin Deutsch für Anfänger.
VOCABULARY SOCCER QUESTIONS PRESENT CONTINOUS WORDLIST – CELL PHONE Unit 2 - Trainer.
Morphology and Syntax More on sentence structure.
Wortstellung (Word Order)
Deutsch 1 G Stunde. Montag, der 10. September 2012 Deutsch 1 (G Stunde)Heute ist ein D - Tag Unit: Introduction to German & Germany Objectives: Introducing.
Meine Schulfächer.
You need to use your mouse to see this presentation © Heidi Behrens.
You need to use your mouse to see this presentation © Heidi Behrens.
INTAKT- Interkulturelle Berufsfelderkundungen als ausbildungsbezogene Lerneinheiten in berufsqualifizierenden Auslandspraktika DE/10/LLP-LdV/TOI/
DEUTSCHLAND UND DIE MEDIEN
DEUTSCHLAND UND DIE MEDIEN
Guten Tag! Mittwoch den Hausaufgabe für Freitag den Finish Unit 3 Vocab Review Prepare Unit 4 Vocab for class.
Verben Wiederholung Deutsch III Notizen.
Blitzkrieg Prompt: Get with your partner number 3: You and a friend from Germany are trying to figure out what to do this coming week. Hold a 1 minute.
Tage der Woche German Early Level Montag Dienstag Mittwoch Donnerstag
Case #1 - Nominative Case
Kölner Karneval By Logan Mack
The word,,aber in German is most often used as a coordinating conjunction. Ich wollte nach Bremen fahren aber Mein Auto ist kaputt. Ich mag English aber.
Einführung Bild und Erkenntnis Einige Probleme Fazit Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen Philosophische Fakultät Institut für Medienwissenschaft Epistemic.
Berner Fachhochschule Hochschule für Agrar-, Forst- und Lebensmittelwissenschaften HAFL Recent activities on ammonia emissions: Emission inventory Rindvieh.
4th Symposium on Lidar Atmospheric Applications
Ein Projekt des Technischen Jugendfreizeit- und Bildungsvereins (tjfbv) e.V. kommunizieren.de Blended Learning for people with disabilities.
The most obvious or direct use of auch is to mean also. Ich möchte auch Gitarre lernen. Auch ich möchte Gitarre lernen. I would like to learn Guitar. Someone.
Grammatik Deutsch I Kapitel 3 – 1. Stufe LERNZIEL:
Nouns. What is a noun O A noun is the name of a person, O place, O animal O or thing.
Talking about yourself
Relativpronomen / Relativsätze:
© Boardworks Ltd of 8 Time Manner Place © Boardworks Ltd of 8 This icon indicates that the slide contains activities created in Flash. These.
By: Jade Bowerman. German numbers are quite a bit like our own. You start with one through ten and then you add 20, 30, 40 or 50 to them. For time you.
German Early Level The Weather.
Adjectiv Endungen Lite: Adjective following articles and pre-ceeding nouns. Colors and Clothes.
Relativpronomen / Relativsätze:
Sentence Structure Subject and verb are always together. Subject and verb are always together. Subject and verb must agree Subject and verb must agree.
To school => zu der Schule With friends => mit den Freunden On top of the desk => auf dem Schreibtisch Through the wall => durch die Wand.
KIT – die Kooperation von Forschungszentrum Karlsruhe GmbH und Universität Karlsruhe (TH) Vorlesung Knowledge Discovery - Institut AIFB Tempus fugit Towards.
German Word Order explained!
Present Tense Most regular verbs follow this pattern:
Separable Verbs Turn to page R22 in your German One Book R22 is in the back of the book There are examples at the top of the page.
1 Stevens Direct Scaling Methods and the Uniqueness Problem: Empirical Evaluation of an Axiom fundamental to Interval Scale Level.
THE PERFECT TENSE IN GERMAN
Adjective Endings Nominative & Accusative Cases describing auf deutsch The information contained in this document may not be duplicated or distributed.
Selectivity in the German Mobility Panel Tobias Kuhnimhof Institute for Transport Studies, University of Karlsruhe Paris, May 20th, 2005.
Technische Universität München 1 CADUI' June FUNDP Namur G B I The FUSE-System: an Integrated User Interface Design Environment Frank Lonczewski.
Instrumente und Unterhaltung End of Unit Assessment.
Mann gegen Man Man / Husband One Mann = man =
Noch most directly has the meaning of still. As such, it has basically for types of usage. The temporal usage of the word. (as it relates to time) can.
By Martin L. Loeffler.  The basic sentence has a subject and a verb.  The subject and verb need to be together.  The subject and verb need to agree.
Andreas Burger ZENTRUM FÜR MEDIZINISCHE LEHRE RUHR-UNIVERSITÄT BOCHUM Irkutsk October 2012 Report about the lecture "Report of the TEMPUS IV- Project Nr.
Warm-up: Kickers ‘ob’, ‘dass’, ‘weil’
THE CONVERSATIONAL PAST
FURTHER MASS SPECTROMETRY
 Präsentation transkript:

Winter symposium Aarhus (DK) 29 -30.1.2007 Wolfgang Wildgen (University of Bremen) Minimal syntax: Comparison of meaning construction in everyday compounds and compounds in experimental poetry (Paul Celan) Winter symposium Aarhus (DK) 29 -30.1.2007

Linguistic and neuro-cognitive prerequisites Part I Linguistic and neuro-cognitive prerequisites

Minimal syntax Simple or even minimal languages are a widespread but not well-defined phenomenon. Roughly speaking, the early stages of language acquisition, e.g. at the one- or two-word level, the foreign language competence of beginners, the pidgins and jargons used for special situations (e.g. bilingual encounters), the language of mentally handicapped persons and aphasics are only specimens out of a large field of reduced language usage. Beyond the human species (Homo sapiens sapiens), the linguistic skills of primates and the reconstructed language of Homo neanderthaliensis or even Homo erectus are possible candidates. The research on the language of primates trained by humans shows us that their language capacities have semantic compositionality, i.e. hey are able to put two (or more) meanings together to form a new complex. We call this minimal syntax.

Nominal compounds and bare nominal groups as candidates for minimal syntax If we take a compound as a kind of utterance per se, outside of its possibly complex syntactic and textual context, its constituents are often conceptually simple (e.g. stems/radicals). Although these morphemes may belong to specific syntactic classes of the language in question, this feature has only a reduced significance for the construction of the meaning of the compound, i.e. the syntactic potential of the morpheme is dormant in the morphological construction. In the case of adjective+ noun and similar constructions, the way of semantic composition is comparable to that of compounds.

Temporal binding as the basic compositional principle The binding process is one of temporal synchronization of neural assemblies, which form wholes (gestalts) from parts and one of desynchronization which distinguishes figure and ground. Cf. Engel (2004) This type of analysis concerns primarily the composition in perception, attentiveness and memory, but one may conjecture a parallel process for words (at least those related to perceptual information) and their composition in syntactic constructions. Temporal binding of neurons could be the equivalent of an object-concept. This is shown in slide 5.

The basic idea of temporal binding Parts or features of a visual whole are linked by the synchronic firing of a set of neurons (an assembly) during a short time interval. In the example the parts and features of the cat and those of the woman are bound together by the internal synchrony of the assemblies 1 and 2 and they are distinguished by the asynchrony of these assemblies. From: Engel et alii, 1997: 572

Ambiguity and Binding Picture a is ambiguous. If it is seen as one face (and a candle in front of it) the zones (1,2) and (3,4) (see series d) are bound; in the case two faces looking at each other are seen, the zones (1,3) und (2,4) are bound. The binding may be recognized by the synchronic firing rates in the series d versus e. From: Engel, Fries und Singer, 2001: 707

Top down effects due to expectation and memory The remembered object produces higher synchronization at the γ-level (30 to 60 Hz) At the left a Kanitza-triangle At the right a non-Kanitza-triangle If the tested person is instructed to recognize the non-Kanitza-triangle , the synchronization is higher for this configuration, although basic gestalt laws would predict the contrary. From: Hermann, Munk und Engel, 2004:349

“ ‘What’ and ‘Where’ are the basic questions to be answered in visual perception. Not only must we recognize what we are looking at, but also we need to know where it is in order to respond appropriately.” Gazzaniga u.a. (1998: 165) If spatial and dynamic aspects on the pathway “Where” are considered, primitive types of processes are combined with simply bound predicates and dynamic gestalts corresponding to what pops up in sentences must be implemented in the brain.

Restrictions on compositionality due to temporal binding „The different firing rates that can be easily discriminated on a background of inherent noise and accidental synchronies may set a low limit to the number of objects that can be simultaneously bound.” Teisman (1999: 108) This restriction is even more dramatic in the case of process-schemata, as the results of catastrophe theory show. These already maximally complex processes exemplify the schemata for exchange (giving) and instrumental action. They contain as subfields the schemata of capture, emission, of beginning, end and stable existence.

List of archetypes with one to three attractors Existing in time and space (one place) Coexistence 8n time and space (two place) Localistic relations: entering, leaving, being in (one place), changing from A to B (two place) changing from A to C via B (three place and further) Affecting (A affects/influences/touches/catches/... B) (two place) Effecting (A ejects/emits/creates/… B) (two place) Instrumentality (A affects C with the instrument B) (three place) Transfer (A gives C to B / B receives C from A) (three place) Causation (A causes C to affect/effect B) (three place)

Nonce-compounds in German One can distinguish three major classes (cf. wildgen, 1982c): Symmetric (almost) compounds: Mann- Frau (,,man-woman") Dichterkomponist (,,poet-composer") Kardinal-Ökonom (,,cardinal-economist") Asymmetric compounds (the vast majority). As a general tendency one can say: generalizing (classificatory) constituents rather stand at the right, evaluative constituents at the left. Historically the first may be become suffixes the latter prefixes: Compounds with constituents containing one or two verbal stems: Arbeits-Begräbnis ("working burial") Horch-Angriff ("listening-attack") Anzapf-Versuch ("attempt to tap") Kontroll-Zwänge ("control restraint")

Nominal compounds in poetic language (Paul Celan) Part II Nominal compounds in poetic language (Paul Celan)

The poet Paul Celan (his family name was Anschel) was born in Czernowitz (Bukowina) in 1920. He went to Paris in 1938 to study medicine but returned to Czernowitz in 1939. He stayed in Rumania until 1947 when he fled to Vienna and 1948 came to Paris to study German Linguistics and Literature. In 1959 he became lecturer (lecteur) of German at the École Normal Supérieure (ENS) in Paris. After 1961 he had to be treated in different psychiatric clinics. He committed suicide in Paris 1971.

Compounds in experimental poetry: analysis of some compounds in poems by Paul Celan (1920-1971) Titles of collections: quantity Mohn und Gedächtnis (1952) 56 poems Von Schwelle zu Schwelle (1955) 47 " Sprachgitter (1959) 31 " Die Niemandsrose (1963) 53 " Atemwende (1967) 80 " Fadensonnen (1968) 105 " Lichtzwang (1970) 81 " Schneepart (1971) 70 " Lichtgehöft (1976) 50 "

Variants of names for collections The collection at left was finally named: Atemwende, the one at the right: Fadensonne.

Other compounds in the Celan corpus Sprachwaage(NR) : language-weighing machine Sprachtürme (AW) : language-towers Sprachnebel (LZ) : language-mist/fog and with Wort (word): Worthöhlen (LZ) : word-caves Wortwaage (NR) : word-weighing machine Wortspur (AW) : word-track/trace Wordwand AW) : word-wall Wortsand (NR) : word-sand Wortwege (NP) : word-lanes Wortlitze (SP) : word-braid Wortschatten (SP) : word-shadow

Nonce-compounds as minimal utterances in titles of poem books by Paul Celan Sprachgitter : SG (language−grid/fence) Niemandsrose : NR (nobody−rose) Atemwende : AW (breath(ing) / −turn / turnabout/reversal) Fadensonnen : FS (thread/fiber/wire−suns) Lichtzwang : LZ (light / candle−compulsion/ necessity/restraint) Schneepart : SP (snow−part) Zeitgehöft : ZG (time−group of farm buildings)

Choice of archetypes as fillers of the relational slot of a nominal compound The construction may exploit generalized relational schemata (cf. section 3) and thus elaborate the basic binding structure (cf. section 2) If one simply takes the list of semantic archetypes with one or two attractors (cf. section 3), one obtains the following relational readings: a fence/grid is/coexists with language/linguistic phenomena a fence/grid becomes language/ a linguistic phenomenon a fence/grid ends/starts language language goes through a fence/grid a fence/grid captures language a fence/grid generates language The compound is compatible with more than one of these; the delimiting case (enter/start) is probably the most significant choice.

Distribution of heavy/central and restrictive/peripheral constituents heavy constituent restrictive constituent Sprachgitter Sprache (language) Gitter (fence) Niemandsrose Rose (rose) Niemand (nobody) Atemwende Atem (breath) Wende (reversal) Fadensonnen Sonne (sun) Faden (thread) Lichtzwang Licht Zwang (compulsion) Scheepart Schnee Part (part) Zeitgehöft Zeit Gehöft (group of farm-buildings)

Proliferation of the type of nonce compound from the titles to the poems In the collection: Atemwende we find: Atemseil (breath-rope) Atemkristall (breath-crystal) Steinatem (stone-breath) In the other collections similar compounds based on breath show up: Niemandsrose : Atembau (breath-fabric) Atemmünze (breath-coin/cash) Schneepart : Atemnot (shortage of breath) The continuous breadth (standing for life/soul) is broken down to limited forms or is lacking (Atemnot)

Further principles Key words of the author, which are statistically prominent (high recurrence) and which sketch the semantic frame of the poetry in question; Simple binary (rarely tertiary) binding patterns, which may be predicted using the hierarchy of semantic archetypes. Some inherent dynamic features of the constituents refer to relations/events and actions: relations :– part (in Schneepart), assembly of farm buildings (Gehöft) – thread (Faden) as opposed to disk (Scheibe) – negation, nobody (niemand) barriers: – fence (´Gitter) antagonistic actions: reversal (Wende), compulsion (Zwang)

Analysis of two poems by Celan Aspen Tree, your leaves glance white into the dark. My mother's hair was never white. Dandelion, so green is the Ukraine. My yellow-haired mother did not come home. Rain cloud, above the well do you hover? My quiet mother weeps for everyone. Round star, you wind the golden loop. My mother's heart was ripped by lead. Oaken door, who lifted you off your hinges? My gentle mother cannot return. The poem Aspen Tree (cf. http://www.penwith.co.uk/artofeurope/poetry.htm).

The poem has five strophes and each one begins with a minimal utterance, either a compound or a noun phrase: Aspen tree (Espenbaum) Dandelion (Löwenzahn = lion tooth) Rain cloud (Regenwolke) Round star (Runder Stern) Oaken door (Eichne Tür) The first line is always completed by a sentence, two of them being questions. The second verse of each strophe (stanza) has “my mother” as recurrent topic.

Todesfuge (fugue of death): Beginning of the poem: 1952 Black milk of daybreak we drink it at nightfall we drink it at noon in the morning we drink it at night drink it and drink it Weiße Milch Sprachgitter (language grid): Beginning of the poem: 1959 Augenrund zwischen den Stäben. Flimmertier Lid rudert nach oben, gibt einen Blick frei. Eye-round Cilium-animal

Analysis of the compounds In the new poems (after Celan’s move to Paris), the compounds are nonce compounds and in general enigmatic: “Todesfuge” (Todestango) refers to a piece of music (Fuge/Tango/ …) and associates it with death. Historically one may reconstruct a coexistence (or precedence) in time of music playing and killing in the concentration camp; but in general the semantic relation remains one of association. “Eye-round” is like a nonce transformation of “round eye” with an inversion of word order. Cilium-animal could be a real animal, but the immediately following “Lid” (lid) shows that the compound describes metaphorically a body part following the schema: body parts are animals. In general very basic types of association like binding processes are used to give meaning to the compounds.

Some conclusions Minimal utterance structure is something underlying not only the grammar of every speech-event, showing up in conversation, in answering questions, in understanding disrupted speech, etc. It also constitutes a cognitive plateau, on which many semiotic processes stand in linguistic, gestured and visual communication. The complexities of lexical knowledge and the intricate difficulties of syntax are like supplementary layers which often hide the basic structures ( as the associative cortex hides the brain stem). In innovative poetic communication this basic plateau shows up very clearly. There are two pathways one may chose to elaborate this plateau:

Augment and make the message more complex by using a more elaborated lexicon (beyond everybody’s active lexicon of ca. 10,000 items). Introduce new or mostly unknown words. Deconstruct ritualized complicated patterns and build up new, yet unusual linguistic patterns. This technique makes the construction of meaning less determined and leaves more choices of interpretation to the audience. It is clear that in order to guarantee some kind of communication between writer and audience, the new systems must contain lawful (predictable) patterns, which can be learned quickly by an experienced audience. Construct a deeper architecture of topics organized in clauses, simple and complex sentences; descriptive, narrative and argumentative chains. These techniques are less adequate in the case of poetry. Add so called poetic super-patterns, such as rime and enforced rhythms (meter). If these have no effect on the construction of meaning they remain decorative.

Bibliography Ballmer, Thomas T. and Wolfgang Wildgen (eds.), 1987. Process Linguistics. Exploring the Processual Aspects of Language and Language Use, and the Methods of their Description, Niemeyer, Tübingen. Celan, Paul, 1983. Gesammelte Werke in fünf Bänden. Dritter Band. Gedichte III. Prosa. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp. Celan, Paul, 2004. Mohn und Gedächtnis. Vorstufen-Textgenese-Endfassung (bearbeitet von Heino Schull unter Mitarbeit von Christiane Braun), Suhrkamp, Frankfurt am Main. Nielsen, Karsten Hoidfelt and Herald Pors, 1981. Index zur Lyrik Celans, Fink, München. Pors, Harald, 1989. Rückläufiges Wortregister zur Lyrik Paul Celans, Fink, München. Fleischer, Michael, 1985. Nomenhäufigkeitsverteilungslisten zur Lyrik von Paul Celan. Statistik der Substantive und ihrer Komposita, Verlag: Die Blaue Eule, Essen.

Gazzaniga, Michael S. , Richard B. Ivry and George R. Mangun, 1998 Gazzaniga, Michael S., Richard B. Ivry and George R. Mangun, 1998. Cognitive Neuroscience. The Biology of the Mind, Norton, New York. Gil, David, 2006a. Early Human Language was Isolating-Monocategorical-Associational, cf. http://www3.isrl.uiuc.edu/~junwang4/langev/localcopy/pdf/gil06evolang.pdf Wildgen, Wolfgang, 1982a. Catastrophe Theoretic Semantics. An Elaboration and Application of René Thom's Theory, Reihe: Pragmatics and Beyond: III, 5, Benjamins, Amsterdam. Wildgen, Wolfgang, 1982b. Makroprozesse bei der Verwendung nominaler ad hoc-Komposita im Deut­schen, in: Deutsche Sprache, 3: 237-257. Wildgen, Wolfgang, 1982c. Zur Dynamik lokaler Kompositionsprozesse. Am Beispiel nominaler ad hoc-Komposita im Deutschen, in: Folia Linguistica, 16: 297-344. Wildgen, Wolfgang, 1985. Archetypensemantik. Grundlagen für eine dynamische Seman­tik auf der Basis der Katastrophentheorie, Reihe: Methoden und Ergebnisse der Sprach­wissenschaft, Narr, Tübingen. Wildgen, Wolfgang, 1987. Dynamic Aspects of Nominal Composition, in: Ballmer und Wildgen, 1987: 128-162.